THEORY
VIDEOS//THEORY
Music Talks: Motive, Tonal Structure, and Form in Beethoven's 'Appassionata' Sonata No. 23, mvt. 1
UNPUBLISHED//THEORY
PUBLISHED//THEORY
Embodied Meaning In John Adams' El Niño
Curriculum//Ear-Training
The following first-year undergraduate ear-training curriculum is one that has been built and implemented successfully by Dr. Bruce Roter, myself, and Kari Francis. It develops the ear in traditional western common-practice, contemporary, and music industry-related sound structures. Areas of development include melodic dictation/transcription, chord progression recognition, harmonic dictation (including voice-leading and harmonies), and rhythmic dictation (one line, as well as two- and three-line drum dictation/transcription.) We are making it open-source, so feel free to make use of it yourself in your undergraduate classroom setting. (Student version and sound files provided here. Instructor copy will be made available on or after May 6, 2021.)
first semester ear-training (student version)
first semester ear-training (audio)
Homeworks 1–7
first semester ear-training (audio)
Homeworks 8–14
first semester ear-training (audio)
Homeworks 15–21
First Semester Ear-Training (Instructor Version)
second semester ear-training (student version)
second semester ear-training (audio)
Homeworks 1–7
second semester ear-training (audio)
Homeworks 8–14
second semester ear-training (audio)
Homeworks 15–21
second Semester Ear-Training (Instructor Version)
Music Theory ideas in incubation (informal notes)
1. Flexible scale-degrees 6 and 7 as
opposed to the 3 standard forms of the
minor scale (a la Gary S.
Karpinski's presentation in the
Manual for Ear Training and Sight
Singing)
2. Triadic cadence names that are more
specific than "progressive" and
"conclusive" when they don't fit into
traditional cadence
nomenclature; subdominant
cadence (ends on IV, iv) supertonic
cadence (ends on ii); supertonic-tonic
cadence (ii–I), etc.
3. Secondary Modal Borrowing: For
example, consider the following harmonic progression: C F#ø7 B7b9 Emaj7...the
progression continues in C major. From the
perspective of E major, where this chord
progression ends, the preceding two
chords can be seen as a iiø7 V7b9 from E
minor (the parallel minor of E major). This is
an instance of modal borrowing; however, it
is from the perspective of a chord which is
not a tonic, but a locally tonicized chord
(which incidentally is not the expected Em
or iii chord in the more global C major).
From this perspective, the Emaj7 is a
Picardy 3rd and maj7 borrowed from E
minor's parallel major. However, Picardy
3rds are usually the purview of tonic
chords, not any and all possible triads. This
situation then, proposes two different
instances of modal borrowing, both with a
"secondary" quality to them. The ii-V is an
instance of secondary modal borrowing
since it is borrowing from the parallel minor
of a tonicized chord. The tonicized chord is
an instance of "secondary" modal
borrowing in the sense that it achieves the
possibility of a Picardy third only as a very
localized tonic rather than a global one.
4. Double function in the cadential
V7sus chord.
5. Melodic Dictation In Pedagogy: Why
Contour Matters In Taking Melodic
Dictation. Some students will insist that
they "got the right note" if, for instance, they
move from sol up to do at the conclusion of
a melodic line when, in fact, it moved
from sol down to do. The pitch class is
correct while the pitch is incorrect and the
contour is incorrect. Would the rest of the
world recognize these types of scenarios
as the "correct" tunes if we took well-known
tunes and completely altered the registers
and directionality of the melodic lines of our
favorite well-known songs? Obviously this
is taking the same concept to the point of absurdity, but by so doing it does help
make the point that we need to be able to
hear directionality as a basic listening
component in melodic
dictation/transcription.
6. Pedagogy: Making Explicit The
Reasons For Note Specificity. Some
students have a tendency to see a teacher
who requires note specificity as the sign of
a bullheaded instructor rather than viewing
such specificity as a practical necessity of
all musicians. For example, a student might
say "I know I said the answer was A C E G,
but of course you know I meant Ab C E Gb.
Same thing. You are so specific." Explicitly
pointing out (and even demonstrating) to
students the musical situations in which the
lack of specificity completely breaks down
in terms of achieving compositional and
performance accuracy which would be
demanded of any musician in any musical
scenario may effectively break students of
such habits.
7. Distinguishing swing at the beat
division level and beat subdivision
level: At the beat division level, the beat is
divided into two unequal parts (long-short)
somewhere between two straight eighths
per beat and straight triplet eighths in
which the first two eighths are tied while the
third is not. At the beat subdivision level,
the divided beat (8th note, assuming a
quarter note beat unit) is divided into two
unequal parts (long-short) somewhere
between two straight 16ths per 8th and
straight triplet 16ths in which the first two
16ths are tied while the third is not.
8. Help! I can't hear intervals! A multi-
contextualization process for
scaffolding the aural recognition of
intervals. An example: The basic idea is
varying small contexts for individual
intervals we are studying. Let's take the
major 2nd for example. I'll use a referent of
C Major.
a. Play the major 2nd melodically, then
harmonically.
b. Play it as an add-note chord: C major, then
Cadd2
c. Play the major 2nd melodically, then
harmonically.
d. Play C major with a doubled root (C-E-G-
C), then Cadd9
e. Play the major 2nd melodically, then
harmonically.
f. Play a whole-tone scale beginning on C
(WT-0)
g. Play the major 2nd melodically, then
harmonically.
h. Play a whole-tone 4-note cluster chord with
a bass note of C.
i. Play the major 2nd melodically, then
harmonically.
j. Play C and D as a M9.
k. Play C4, D5, C5 melodically.
l. Play C4, D4, C4 melodically.
m. Play C4, D4, C4, Bb3 melodically to orient
it to a different key context (new tonal
center.)
n. Play a Gsus4 (G-C-D) going to G major (G-
B-D) *4-3 suspension.
o. Play Gsus 4 without resolving it.
p. Play a major 9 (C-D)
q. Play Cmaj9
r. Play a major 9 (C-D)
s. Play C9
t. Play a major 9 (C-D)
u. Play a C-9
v. Play a major 9 (C-D)
w. Play a Cø9
x. Play a major 9 (C-D)
y. Play a Cº9
z. Play a major 9.
aa. Play a Csus2, then C (CEG)
In this way, we can help them not be so "hit or
miss" with intervals, but scaffold their listening
experience through small musical contexts for those intervals. We can simultaneously
introduce them to sounds (both traditional and
contemporary) that make use of those
intervals.
9. Measuring Strength Of, And
Conformance To, Tonic Perception In
Melody: Create various melodies intentionally
crafted with the intent of measuring how much
listener conformance to (or deviation from) the
perception of a particular tonic(s) occur(s)
depending upon the structure of a given
melody.
10. Examples Of Negative Musical Space
In Pitch And Rhythm (and Timbre?):
This idea comes from Bert Ligon in his
textbook entitled Jazz Theory Resources (p.
23) where he states "Negative space is
implied space created by a positive image.
Visual artists depend on the recognition of
negative space. The concept of negative
space is also important in music. Any pitches
that are played (positive space) may imply
other pitches that are not played. A certain
pitch may be stressed by playing a number
of pitches around that pitch that point to that
pitch, while never actually playing the pitch.
...As with pitches, any rhythm that is played
(positive space) may imply a rhythm not
played (negative space)." Perhaps an
example would be a ii-V which implies a
certain tonic resolution, but never actually
provides it. Another example could be
running a major scale from low do up to
high ti and then simply stopping. In certain
contexts, we may feel an implication
that ti implies a resolution to do, whether or
not this implication is actualized. When these
types of implication are not realized, they are
examples of negative musical space.